User:Finereach/Social Cognition

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Social cognition is the encoding, storage, retrieval, and processing, in the brain, of information relating to conspecifics, or members of the same species. At one time social cognition referred specifically to an approach to social psychology in which these processes were studied according to the methods of cognitive psychology and information processing theory. However the term has come to be more widely used across psychology and cognitive neuroscience. For example, "social cognition" is used to refer to various social abilities disrupted in autism[1] and other disorders[2]. In cognitive neuroscience the biological basis of social cognition is investigated[3][4][5]. Developmental psychologists study the development of social cognition abilities[6].

Social cognition as a field of social psychology[edit]

Social cognition came to prominence with the rise of cognitive psychology in the late 1960s and early 1970s, and referred to an eponymous field of study. It is now the dominant model and approach in mainstream social psychology.[citation needed] Common to social cognition theories within social psychology is the idea that information is represented in the brain as "cognitive elements" - such as schemas, attributions, or stereotypes - and a focus on how these cognitive elements are processed. Social cognition therefore applies and extends many themes, theories and paradigms from cognitive psychology, for example in reasoning (representativeness heuristic, base rate fallacy and confirmation bias), attention (automaticity and priming) and memory (schemas, primacy and recency). It is very likely that social psychology was always a lot more cognitive than mainstream psychology to begin with, as it traditionally discussed internal mental states such as beliefs and desires when mainstream psychology was dominated by behaviourism and rejected them as illusory [7]. A parallel paradigm has arisen in the study of action, termed motor cognition [1]. Motor cognition is concerned with understanding the representation of action and the associated process.

In social psychology, a notable theory of social cognition is social schema theory, although this is not the basis of all studies of social cognition. For example, attribution theory is an alternative theory in social psychology[7] and it has been suggested that other disciplines in social psychology such as social identity theory and social representations may be seeking to explain largely the same phenomena as social cognition and that these different disciplines might be merged into a "coherent integrated whole"[8].

Social cognition is now widely used outside social psychology as described in later sections.

Social schemas[edit]

Social schema theory builds on and uses terminology from schema theory in cognitive psychology which describes how ideas, or “concepts” from the world around us are represented in the brain, and how they are categorised. According to this view, when we see or think of a concept, a mental representation or schema is "activated", bringing to mind other information which is linked to the original concept by association. This activation often happens unconsciously. As a result of activating such schemas judgements are formed which go beyond the information actually available. This may influence thinking and social behavior regardless of whether these judgements are accurate or not. For example, if an individual is introduced as a teacher, a "teacher schema" may be activated and we might associate this person with wisdom or authority.

When a schema is more accessible this means it can more quickly be activated and used in a particular situation. Two cognitive processes that increase the accessibility of schemas are salience and priming . Salience is the degree to which a particular social object stands out relative to other social objects in a situation. The higher the salience of an object the more likely that schemas for that object will be made accessible. For example, if there is one female in a group of seven males, female gender schemas may be more accessible and influence the group’s thinking and behavior toward the female group member[7]. Priming refers to any experience immediately prior to a situation that cause a schema to be more accessible. For example watching a scary movie at a theatre late at night might increase the accessibility of frightening schemas that affect a person’s perception of shadows and background noises as potential threats.

Social cognition researchers are also interested in how new information is integrated into pre-established schemas, especially when that information is contrary with those pre-established schemas. Pre-established schemas tend to guide attention to new information. People selectively attend to information that is consistent with the schema and ignore information that is inconsistent. This is referred to as a confirmation bias. Sometimes inconsistent information is sub-categorized and stored away as a special case, leaving the original schema intact without any alterations. This is referred to as subtyping.

Social cognition researchers are also interested in studying the regulation of activated schemas. It is believed that the situational activation of schemas is automatic, meaning that it is outside the control of the individual. In many situations however, the schematic information that has been activated may be in conflict with the social norms of situation, in which case an individual is motivated to inhibit the influence of the schematic information on their thinking and social behavior. Whether a person will successfully regulate the application of the activated schemas is dependent on individual differences in self-regulatory ability and the presence of situational impairments to executive control. High self-regulatory ability and the lack of situational impairments on executive functioning increase the likelihood that individuals will successfully inhibit the influence of automatically activated schemas on their thinking and social behavior. However, when people stop suppressing the influence of the unwanted thoughts, a rebound effect can occur where the thought becomes hyper-accessible.

Relation to cognitive neuroscience[edit]

Neuropsychology[edit]

Early interest in the relationship between brain function and social cognition includes the case of Phineas Gage, whose behavior was reported to have changed after an accident damaged one or both of his frontal lobes. More recent neuropsychological studies have shown that brain injury disrupts social cognitive processes. For example, damage to the frontal lobes can affect emotional responses to social stimuli[9] [10] [11] , performance on social reasoning tasks [12] and performance on Theory of Mind tasks [13][14]. In the temporal lobe damage to the fusiform gyrus can lead to the inability to recognize faces.

Psychological disorders[edit]

People with psychological disorders such as autism[1], Williams syndrome, Antisocial Personality Disorder[2], Fragile X and Turner's syndrome[15] show differences in social behavior compared to their unaffected peers. However, whether social cognition is underpinned by domain specific neural mechanisms is still an open issue.[16]

There is now an expanding research field examining how such conditions may bias cognitive processes involved in social interaction, or conversely, how such biases may lead to the symptoms associated with the condition.

Relation to other fields of psychology[edit]

Development[edit]

Social cognition develops during infancy, childhood and adolescence[6]. It has been suggested that some aspects of psychological processes that promote social behavior (such as face recognition) may be innate. Consistent with this very young babies recognize and selectively respond to social stimuli such as the voice, face and scent of their mother.[17] Social perception continues to develop throughout infancy.

Theory of mind refers to the ability to understand what other people think, feel, known and understand. At four years children are able to demonstrating that they know that someone else can hold a false belief. Theory of mind abilities become more sophisticated as children get older, for example, able to complete more complicated second-order theory of mind tasks at around seven[citation needed] years of age[18]. Ability to understand theory of mind is disrupted in autism.

An outstanding question is the extent to which this development results from general or socially specific cognitive development processes. Some psychologists explain social cognitive development by reference to theories of general cognitive development , for example, Piaget's Theory of cognitive development[6]. According to this view, social cognitive abilities improve in line with ability to perform increasingly complex cognitive operations. For example, as object permanence develops, babies also come to understand "person permanence". Piaget also considered children to be initially egocentric, unable to consider how others see things until a certain stage of cognitive development.

Selman's role-taking theory is another approach to understanding social cognitive development[19]. In this theory, social cognition refers to children's ability to understand other peoples thoughts, desires, motives and behaviour. This stage theory suggests that development of social cognition arises from children's ability to see things from other people's perspectives. Role-taking theory describes development from a simplistic egocentric perspect at 3-6 years, to a more sophisticated general model of how other people behave at 12-15 years[6]. Role-taking theory has been used to develop a clinal intervention for children and adolescents who have have difficulties with social ability[20].

Perception[edit]

Social perception refers to the specialisation or tuning of perceptual systems for processing social cues, including phenomena such as face perception, biological motion and joint attention. Single cell recording studies demonstrate neurons which respond differently to social stimuli. These include mirror neurons, and neurons that respond to faces, and moving images of socially significant behaviour - for example rapidly establishing changes in gaze direction[21].

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b Striano, T; Reid, V (eds.). Social Cognition: Development, Neuroscience and Autism. WileyBlackwell. ISBN 1405162171.
  2. ^ a b Blair, J (2005). Psychopathy, emotion and the brain. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 25–27. ISBN 0631233369. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  3. ^ Cacioppo, J.T., Berntson, G.G., Sheridan, J.F., & McClintock, M.K. (2000). "Multilevel integrative analyses of human behavior: social neuroscience and the complementing nature of social and biological approaches." Psychological Bulletin, 126, 829-843.
  4. ^ Cacioppo, J.T. (2002). Social neuroscience: understanding the pieces fosters understanding the whole and vice versa. American Psychologist, 57, 819-831.
  5. ^ Adolphs, R (1999). "Social cognition and the human brain". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 3: 469–79.
  6. ^ a b c d Shaffer, DR (2009). "Chapter 12: Theories of social and cognitive development". Developmental Psychology: Childhood and Adolescence. Wadsworth Publishing Company. ISBN 0495601713. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help) Cite error: The named reference "Shaffer" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  7. ^ a b c Fiske, ST (1991). Social Cognition. McGraw-Hill, Inc. ISBN 0071009108. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  8. ^ Augustinos, M (2006). Social Cognition an Integrated Introduction. London: Sage Publications Ltd. ISBN 0-7619-4218-1. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  9. ^ Harmon-Jones, E (2007). Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior. Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1-59385-404-1. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  10. ^ Damasio, AR (1994). Descarte's Error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Picador. ISBN 0333656563. {{cite book}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help)
  11. ^ Hornak, J (1996). "Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioral changes following ventral frontal lobe damage". Neuropsychologia. 34: 247–61. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(95)00106-9. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  12. ^ Cosmides, L (2000). "The cognitive neuroscience of social reasoning". In Gazzaniga, MS (ed.) (ed.). The New Cognitive Neurosciences. pp. 1259–70. {{cite book}}: |editor= has generic name (help); Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  13. ^ Stone, VE (1998). "Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 10: 640–656. doi:10.1162/089892998562942. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  14. ^ Brunet, E (2000). "A PET investigation of attribution of intentions to others with a non-verbal task". NeuroImage. 11: 157–166. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  15. ^ Mazzocco, MMM (1998). "Social Functioning Among Girls with Fragile X or Turner Syndrome and Their Sisters". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 28 (6): 509–517. doi:10.1023/A:1026000111467. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  16. ^ Stone, VE (2006). "What's domain-specific about theory of mind". Social Neuroscience. 1 (3–4): 309–319. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  17. ^ Bremner, JG (1994). "Chapter 5 : Social Development". Infancy. pp. 182–183..
  18. ^ Cite error: The named reference BaronCohen was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  19. ^ "Academia: Selman's Theory of Social Role Taking". 20100108. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  20. ^ Coleman, JC (1999). The Nature of Adolescence. London: Routledge. ISBN 0-415-19897-6. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauther= ignored (help)
  21. ^ Brothers, L (1997). Friday's Footprint How Society Shapes the Human Mind. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0195101030.

Further reading[edit]

  • Fiske, S.T. (2004). Social beings: A core motives approach to social psychology. New York: Wiley.
  • Malle, B. (2004). How the mind explains behavior: Folk explanations, meaning, and social interaction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Malle, B., & Hodges, S.D. (2005). Other Minds: How Humans Bridge the Divide Between Self and Others. New York: Guilford Press.
  • Valsiner, J., ‘Social organization of cognitive development, Internalization and externalization of constraint systems,’ In Demetriou, et al., (eds.), Neo-Piagetian Theories of Cognitive Development (New York, Routledge 1992).